Time preferences and their life outcome correlates: Evidence from a representative survey.

Item request has been placed! ×
Item request cannot be made. ×
loading   Processing Request
  • Author(s): Horn D;Horn D;Horn D; Kiss HJ; Kiss HJ; Kiss HJ
  • Source:
    PloS one [PLoS One] 2020 Jul 30; Vol. 15 (7), pp. e0236486. Date of Electronic Publication: 2020 Jul 30 (Print Publication: 2020).
  • Publication Type:
    Journal Article; Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov't
  • Language:
    English
  • Additional Information
    • Source:
      Publisher: Public Library of Science Country of Publication: United States NLM ID: 101285081 Publication Model: eCollection Cited Medium: Internet ISSN: 1932-6203 (Electronic) Linking ISSN: 19326203 NLM ISO Abbreviation: PLoS One Subsets: MEDLINE
    • Publication Information:
      Original Publication: San Francisco, CA : Public Library of Science
    • Subject Terms:
    • Abstract:
      We collect data on time preferences of a representative sample of the Hungarian adult population in a non-incentivized way and investigate how patience and present bias associate with important life outcomes in five domains: i) educational attainment, ii) unemployment, iii) income and wealth, iv) financial decisions and difficulties, and v) health. Based on the literature, we formulate the broad hypotheses that patience relates positively, while present bias associates negatively with positive outcomes in the domains under study. With the exception of unemployment, we document a consistent and often significant positive relationship between patience and the corresponding domain, with the strongest associations in educational attainment, wealth and financial decisions. We find that present bias associates significantly with saving decisions and financial difficulties.
      Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.
    • References:
      Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2016 Nov 22;113(47):13354-13359. (PMID: 27830648)
      Psychol Sci. 2015 Jun;26(6):709-23. (PMID: 25870404)
      Psychol Sci. 2012 Jan 1;23(1):56-8. (PMID: 22157518)
      Econ Hum Biol. 2006 Jan;4(1):39-61. (PMID: 16364701)
      Appetite. 2008 Nov;51(3):563-9. (PMID: 18513828)
      Am Econ Rev. 2013 Apr;103(2):690-731. (PMID: 29524925)
      J Econ Perspect. 2018;32(2):115-34. (PMID: 30203932)
      J Gen Psychol. 2010 Jul-Sep;137(3):273-86. (PMID: 20718227)
      Psychol Bull. 2004 Sep;130(5):769-92. (PMID: 15367080)
      J Biosoc Sci. 2004 Mar;36(2):209-19. (PMID: 15030016)
      J Risk Uncertain. 2008 Dec 1;37(2-3):237-269. (PMID: 19412359)
      Econ Inq. 2011;49(4):959-72. (PMID: 22165417)
      Med Decis Making. 2010 Jan-Feb;30(1):99-112. (PMID: 19675322)
      PLoS One. 2018 Feb 16;13(2):e0190163. (PMID: 29451886)
      Am Econ Rev. 2016 Oct;106(10):3064-3103. (PMID: 28781375)
      Am Econ Rev. 2016 Feb;106(2):260-284. (PMID: 28003681)
      J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 1996 May;22(3):771-91. (PMID: 8656156)
      J Health Econ. 2007 Sep 1;26(5):927-49. (PMID: 17574694)
      Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011 Feb 15;108(7):2693-8. (PMID: 21262822)
      Psychol Sci. 2002 May;13(3):219-24. (PMID: 12009041)
      Psychol Sci. 2005 Dec;16(12):939-44. (PMID: 16313657)
      J Educ Psychol. 2012 May 1;104(2):439-451. (PMID: 24072936)
      Am J Psychol. 1962 Sep;75:485-91. (PMID: 13881416)
      J Risk Uncertain. 2018;56(2):117-140. (PMID: 31007384)
      J Exp Psychol Gen. 2009 Aug;138(3):329-40. (PMID: 19653793)
      J Econ Lit. 2020 Jun;58(2):299-347. (PMID: 37691693)
      Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2013 Jul 9;110(28):11267-71. (PMID: 23798401)
      J Risk Uncertain. 2018 Oct;57(2):177-198. (PMID: 31244508)
    • Publication Date:
      Date Created: 20200731 Date Completed: 20200922 Latest Revision: 20240329
    • Publication Date:
      20240329
    • Accession Number:
      PMC7392281
    • Accession Number:
      10.1371/journal.pone.0236486
    • Accession Number:
      32730296