A transdisciplinary view on curiosity beyond linguistic humans: animals, infants, and artificial intelligence.

Item request has been placed! ×
Item request cannot be made. ×
loading   Processing Request
  • Additional Information
    • Source:
      Publisher: Cambridge University Press Country of Publication: England NLM ID: 0414576 Publication Model: Print-Electronic Cited Medium: Internet ISSN: 1469-185X (Electronic) Linking ISSN: 00063231 NLM ISO Abbreviation: Biol Rev Camb Philos Soc Subsets: MEDLINE
    • Publication Information:
      Original Publication: London, Cambridge University Press.
    • Subject Terms:
    • Abstract:
      Curiosity is a core driver for life-long learning, problem-solving and decision-making. In a broad sense, curiosity is defined as the intrinsically motivated acquisition of novel information. Despite a decades-long history of curiosity research and the earliest human theories arising from studies of laboratory rodents, curiosity has mainly been considered in two camps: 'linguistic human' and 'other'. This is despite psychology being heritable, and there are many continuities in cognitive capacities across the animal kingdom. Boundary-pushing cross-disciplinary debates on curiosity are lacking, and the relative exclusion of pre-linguistic infants and non-human animals has led to a scientific impasse which more broadly impedes the development of artificially intelligent systems modelled on curiosity in natural agents. In this review, we synthesize literature across multiple disciplines that have studied curiosity in non-verbal systems. By highlighting how similar findings have been produced across the separate disciplines of animal behaviour, developmental psychology, neuroscience, and computational cognition, we discuss how this can be used to advance our understanding of curiosity. We propose, for the first time, how features of curiosity could be quantified and therefore studied more operationally across systems: across different species, developmental stages, and natural or artificial agents.
      (© 2024 The Authors. Biological Reviews published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd on behalf of Cambridge Philosophical Society.)
    • References:
      Addyman, C. & Mareschal, D. (2013). Local redundancy governs infants' spontaneous orienting to visual‐temporal sequences. Child Development 84(4), 1137–1144.
      Ahmadlou, M., Houba, J. H., van Vierbergen, J. F., Giannouli, M., Gimenez, G. A., van Weeghel, C., Darbbanfouladi, M., Yasamin Shirazi, M., Dziubek, J., Kacem, M. d., Winter, F. & Heimel, J. A. (2021). A cell type–specific cortico‐subcortical brain circuit for investigatory and novelty‐seeking behavior. Science 372(6543), eabe9681.
      Andrews, K. (2020). The Animal Mind: An Introduction to the Philosophy of Animal Cognition. Routledge, Abingdon.
      Bacon, E. S. (1980). Curiosity in the American black bear. In Bear Biology Association Conference Series (Volume 4), pp. 153–157. Jstore, Montana.
      Baer, C. & Kidd, C. (2022). Learning with certainty in childhood. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 26(10), 887–896.
      Baillargeon, R. (2008). Innate ideas revisited: for a principle of persistence in infants' physical reasoning. Perspectives on Psychological Science 3(1), 2–13.
      Baldassarre, G. (2011). What are intrinsic motivations? A biological perspective. In 2011 IEEE International Conference on Development and Learning (ICDL), pp. 1–8. Frankfurt am Main, Germany.
      Baldassarre, G. & Mirolli, M. (eds) (2013). Intrinsically Motivated Learning in Natural and Artificial Systems, pp. 1–14. Springer, Berlin Heidelberg.
      Baranes, A. & Oudeyer, P. Y. (2013). Active learning of inverse models with intrinsically motivated goal exploration in robots. Robotics and Autonomous Systems 61, 49–73.
      Barrett, L. P., Stanton, L. A. & Benson‐Amram, S. (2019). The cognition of ‘nuisance'species. Animal Behaviour 147, 167–177.
      Barto, A. G. (2013). Intrinsic motivation and reinforcement learning. In Intrinsically Motivated Learning in Natural and Artificial Systems, pp. 17–47. Springer Berlin, Heidelberg.
      Barto, A. G. & Simsek, O. (2005). Intrinsic motivation for reinforcement learning systems. In Proceedings of the Thirteenth Yale Workshop on Adaptive and Learning Systems, pp. 113–118. Yale University Press, New Haven, CO.
      Bashaw, M. J., Gibson, M. D., Schowe, D. M. & Kucher, A. S. (2016). Does enrichment improve reptile welfare? Leopard geckos (Eublepharis macularius) respond to five types of environmental enrichment. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 184, 150–160.
      Bazhydai, M., Westermann, G. & Parise, E. (2020). ‘I don't know but I know who to ask’: 12‐month‐olds actively seek information from knowledgeable adults. Developmental Science 23(5), e12938.
      Begus, K. & Bonawitz, E. (2020). The rhythm of learning: theta oscillations as an index of active learning in infancy. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience 45, 100810.
      Begus, K., Gliga, T. & Southgate, V. (2014). Infants learn what they want to learn: responding to infant pointing leads to superior learning. PLoS One 9(10), e108817.
      Begus, K. & Southgate, V. (2012). Infant pointing serves an interrogative function. Developmental Science 15(5), 611–617.
      Begus, K. & Southgate, V. (2018). Curious learners: how infants' motivation to learn shapes and is shaped by infants' interactions with the social world. In Active Learning from Infancy to Childhood: Social Motivation, Cognition, and Linguistic Mechanisms, pp. 13–37. Springer International Publishing AG, Cham.
      Beiser, H. R. (1984). On curiosity: a developmental approach. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry 23(5), 517–526.
      Bellemare, M., Srinivasan, S., Ostrovski, G., Schaul, T., Saxton, D. & Munos, R. (2016). Unifying count‐based exploration and intrinsic motivation. Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems 29, 1471–1479.
      Benedict, B. M. (2001). Curiosity: A Cultural History of Early Modern Inquiry. University of Chicago Press, Chicago.
      Benson‐Amaram, S., Weldele, M. L. & Holekamp, K. E. (2013). A comparison of innovative problem‐solving abilities between wild and captive spotted hyaenas, Crocuta crocuta. Animal Behaviour 85(2), 349–356.
      Bergman, T. J. & Kitchen, D. M. (2009). Comparing responses to novel objects in wild baboons (Papio ursinus) and geladas (Theropithecus gelada). Animal Cognition 12, 63–73.
      Bering, J. M. (2004). A critical review of the ‘enculturation hypothesis’: the effects of human rearing on great ape social cognition. Animal Cognition 7, 201–212.
      Berlyne, D. E. (1950). Novelty and curiosity as determinants of exploratory behaviour. British Journal of Psychology 41(1), 68.
      Berlyne, D. E. (1954). An experimental study of human curiosity. British Journal of Psychology 45(4), 256.
      Berlyne, D. E. (1955). The arousal and satiation of perceptual curiosity in the rat. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 48(4), 238–246.
      Berlyne, D. E. (1960). Conflict, Arousal, and Curiosity. McGraw‐Hill Book Company, New York.
      Berlyne, D. E. (1962). Uncertainty and epistemic curiosity. British Journal of Psychology 53(1), 27–34.
      Berlyne, D. E. (1966). Curiosity and exploration: animals spend much of their time seeking stimuli whose significance raises problems for psychology. Science 153(3731), 25–33.
      Bevins, R. A. & Besheer, J. (2006). Object recognition in rats and mice: a one‐trial non‐matching‐to‐sample learning task to study recognition memory. Nature Protocols 1(3), 1306–1311.
      Birchmeier, K., Johnson‐Ulrich, L., Stein, J. & Forss, S. (2023). The role of umwelt in animal curiosity: a within and between species comparison of novelty exploration in mongooses. Animal Behaviour and Cognition 10(4), 329–354.
      Bisazza, A., Lippolis, G. & Vallortigara, G. (2001). Lateralization of ventral fins use during object exploration in the blue gourami (Trichogaster trichopterus). Physiology & Behavior 72(4), 575–578.
      Blanchard, T. C., Hayden, B. Y. & Bromberg‐Martin, E. S. (2015). Orbitofrontal cortex uses distinct codes for different choice attributes in decisions motivated by curiosity. Neuron 85(3), 602–614.
      Blaser, R. & Heyser, C. (2015). Spontaneous object recognition: a promising approach to the comparative study of memory. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience 9, 183.
      Bonawitz, E. B., van Schijndel, T. J., Friel, D. & Schulz, L. (2012). Children balance theories and evidence in exploration, explanation, and learning. Cognitive Psychology 64(4), 215–234.
      Botvinick, M. M., Niv, Y. & Barto, A. G. (2009). Hierarchically organized behavior and its neural foundations: a reinforcement learning perspective. Cognition 113(3), 262–280.
      Bromberg‐Martin, E. S. & Hikosaka, O. (2009). Midbrain dopamine neurons signal preference for advance information about upcoming rewards. Neuron 63(1), 119–126.
      Brown, G. R. & Nemes, C. (2008). The exploratory behaviour of rats in the hole‐board apparatus: is head‐dipping a valid measure of neophilia? Behavioural Processes 78(3), 442–448.
      Burda, Y., Edwards, H., Pathak, D., Storkey, A., Darrell, T. & Efros, A. A. (2018). Large‐scale study of curiosity‐driven learning. arXiv preprint arXiv:1808.04355.
      Bush, G., Luu, P. & Posner, M. I. (2000). Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 4(6), 215–222.
      Butler, R. A. (1954). Curiosity in monkeys. Scientific American 190(2), 70–75.
      Byrne, R. W. (2013). Animal curiosity. Current Biology 23(11), 469–470.
      Carlson, B. E. & Langkilde, T. (2013). Personality traits are expressed in bullfrog tadpoles during open‐field trials. Journal of Herpetology 47(2), 378–383.
      Carruthers, P. (2018). Basic questions. Mind & Language 33, 130–147.
      Carter, A. J., Forss, S., Page, R. A. & Ratcliffe, J. M. (2018). Younger vampire bats (Desmodus rotundus) are more likely than adults to explore novel objects. PLoS One 13(5), e0196889.
      Cesana‐Arlotti, N., Martin, A., Teglas, E., Vorobyova, L., Cetnarski, R. & Bonatti, L. L. (2018). Precursors of logical reasoning in preverbal human infants. Science 359(6381), 1263–1266.
      Clark, F. E., Greggor, A. L., Montgomery, S. H. & Plotnik, J. M. (2023). The endangered brain: actively preserving ex‐situ animal behaviour and cognition will benefit in‐situ conservation. Royal Society Open Science 10(8), 230707.
      Colas, C., Karch, T., Sigaud, O. & Oudeyer, P. Y. (2020). Intrinsically motivated goal‐conditioned reinforcement learning: a short survey. arXiv preprint arXiv:2012.09830.
      Cooper, R. P. & Aslin, R. N. (1990). Preference for infant‐directed speech in the first month after birth. Child Development 61(5), 1584–1595.
      Coughlin, C., Hembacher, E., Lyons, K. E. & Ghetti, S. (2015). Introspection on uncertainty and judicious help‐seeking during the preschool years. Developmental Science 18(6), 957–971.
      Dacey, M. (2017). Anthropomorphism as cognitive bias. Philosophy of Science 84(5), 1152–1164.
      Daddaoua, N., Lopes, M. & Gottlieb, J. (2016). Intrinsically motivated oculomotor exploration guided by uncertainty reduction and conditioned reinforcement in non‐human primates. Scientific Reports 6(1), 1–15.
      Damerius, L. A., Forss, S. I. F., Kosonen, Z. K., Willems, E. P., Burkart, J. M., Call, J., Galdikas, B. M. F., Liebal, K., Haun, D. B. M. & van Schaik, C. P. (2017a). Orientation toward humans predicts cognitive performance in orang‐utans. Scientific Reports 7(1), 1–12.
      Damerius, L. A., Graber, S. M., Willems, E. P. & van Schaik, C. P. (2017b). Curiosity boosts orang‐utan problem‐solving ability. Animal Behaviour 134, 57–70.
      Davis, R. T., Settlage, P. H. & Harlow, H. F. (1950). Performance of normal and brain‐operated monkeys on mechanical puzzles with and without food incentive. The Pedagogical Seminary and Journal of Genetic Psychology 77(2), 305–311.
      Day, H. I. (1982). Curiosity and the interested explorer. Performance & Instruction. 21, 19–22.
      Day, R. L., Coe, R. L., Kendal, J. R. & Laland, K. N. (2003). Neophilia, innovation and social learning: a study of intergeneric differences in callitrichid monkeys. Animal Behaviour 65(3), 559–571.
      Dayan, P. & Balleine, B. W. (2002). Reward, motivation, and reinforcement learning. Neuron 36, 285–298.
      De Lange, M., Aljundi, R., Masana, M., Parisot, S., Jia, X., Leonardis, A., Slabaugh, G. & Tuytelaars, T. (2021). A continual learning survey: defying forgetting in classification tasks. IEEE Transactions on Pattern Analysis and Machine Intelligence 44(7), 3366–3385.
      De Pisapia, N., Bacci, F. & Melcher, D. (2016). Brain networks for visual creativity: a functional connectivity study of planning a visual artwork. Scientific Reports 6(1), 39185.
      Dean, V., Tulsiani, S. & Gupta, A. (2020). See, hear, explore: curiosity via audio‐visual association. Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems 33, 14961–14972.
      Deci, E. L. & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic Motivation and Self‐Determination in Human Behavior. Springer Science & Business Media, New York.
      Emberson, L. L., Cannon, G., Palmeri, H., Richards, J. E. & Aslin, R. N. (2017). Using fNIRS to examine occipital and temporal responses to stimulus repetition in young infants: evidence of selective frontal cortex involvement. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience 23, 26–38.
      Fantz, R. L. (1964). Visual experience in infants: decreased attention to familiar patterns relative to novel ones. Science 146(3644), 668–670.
      Farroni, T., Johnson, M. H., Menon, E., Zulian, L., Faraguna, D. & Csibra, G. (2005). Newborns' preference for face‐relevant stimuli: effects of contrast polarity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 102(47), 17245–17250.
      Ferdowsian, H. (2011). Human and animal research guidelines: aligning ethical constructs with new scientific developments. Bioethics 25(8), 472–478.
      Fernald, A. & Kuhl, P. (1987). Acoustic determinants of infant preference for motherese speech. Infant Behavior and Development 10(3), 279–293.
      Forestier, S., Portelas, R., Mollard, Y. & Oudeyer, P. Y. (2022). Intrinsically motivated goal exploration processes with automatic curriculum learning. The Journal of Machine Learning Research 23(1), 6818–6858.
      Forss, S. & Willems, E. (2022). The curious case of great ape curiosity and how it is shaped by sociality. Ethology 128(8), 552–563.
      Forss, S. I. F., Koski, S. E. & van Schaik, C. P. (2017). Explaining the paradox of neophobic explorers: the social information hypothesis. International Journal of Primatology 38, 799–822.
      Forss, S. I. F., Motes‐Rodrigo, A., Dongre, P., Mohr, T. & van de Waal, E. (2022). Captivity and habituation to humans raise curiosity in vervet monkeys. Animal Cognition 25, 1–12.
      Forss, S. I. F., Schuppli, C., Haiden, D., Zweifel, N. & van Schaik, C. P. (2015). Contrasting responses to novelty by wild and captive orangutans. American Journal of Primatology 77(10), 1109–1121.
      Fox, R. A. & Millam, J. R. (2007). Novelty and individual differences influence neophobia in orange‐winged Amazon parrots (Amazona amazonica). Applied Animal Behaviour Science 104(1–2), 107–115.
      Franks, B. (2019). What do animals want? Animal Welfare 28(1), 1–10.
      Frazier, B. N., Gelman, S. A. & Wellman, H. M. (2009). Preschoolers' search for explanatory information within adult–child conversation. Child Development 80(5), 1592–1611.
      Friston, K. J., Lin, M., Frith, C. D., Pezzulo, G., Hobson, J. A. & Ondobaka, S. (2017). Active inference, curiosity and insight. Neural Computation 29(10), 2633–2683.
      Fritz, R. G., Zimmermann, E., Meier, M., Mestre‐Frances, N., Radespiel, U. & Schmidtke, D. (2020). Neurobiological substrates of animal personality and cognition in a nonhuman primate (Microcebus murinus). Brain and Behavior 10(9), e01752.
      Gallagher, M. W. & Lopez, S. J. (2007). Curiosity and well‐being. The Journal of Positive Psychology 2(4), 236–248.
      Gergely, G., Nadasdy, Z., Csibra, G. & Biro, S. (1995). Taking the intentional stance at 12 months of age. Cognition 56(2), 165–193.
      Glickman, S. E. & Hartz, K. E. (1964). Exploratory behavior in several species of rodents. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 58(1), 101–104.
      Glickman, S. E. & Sroges, R. W. (1966). Curiosity in zoo animals. Behaviour 26(1–2), 151–187.
      Golman, R. & Loewenstein, G. (2018). Information gaps: a theory of preferences regarding the presence and absence of information. Decision 5(3), 143–164.
      Gopnik, A., Meltzoff, A. N. & Kuhl, P. K. (1999). The Scientist in the Crib: Minds, Brains, and how Children Learn. William Morrow & Co, New York.
      Gottfredson, L. S. (1998). The general intelligence factor. Scientific American 9, 24–30.
      Gottlieb, J., Lopes, M. & Oudeyer, P. Y. (2016). Motivated cognition: neural and computational mechanisms of curiosity, attention, and intrinsic motivation. In Recent Developments in Neuroscience Research on Human Motivation. Emerald Group Publishing Limited, Leeds.
      Gottlieb, J., Oudeyer, P. Y., Lopes, M. & Baranes, A. (2013). Information‐seeking, curiosity, and attention: computational and neural mechanisms. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 17(11), 585–593.
      Goupil, L. & Kouider, S. (2016). Behavioral and neural indices of metacognitive sensitivity in preverbal infants. Current Biology 26, 3038–3045.
      Goupil, L. & Proust, J. (2023). Curiosity as a metacognitive feeling. Cognition 231, 105325.
      Goupil, L., Romand‐Monnier, M. & Kouider, S. (2016). Infants ask for help when they know they don't know. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 113(13), 3492–3496.
      Greenberg, R. S. (1990). Ecological plasticity, neophobia, and resource use in birds. Studies in Avian Biology 13, 431–437.
      Greenberg, R. S. (2003). The role of neophobia and neophilia in the development of innovative behaviour of birds. In Animal innovation (eds S. Reader and K. Laland). Oxford University Press, Oxford.
      Greggor, A. L., Jolles, J. W., Thornton, A. & Clayton, N. S. (2016). Seasonal changes in neophobia and its consistency in rooks: the effect of novelty type and dominance position. Animal Behaviour 121, 11–20.
      Greggor, A. L., Thornton, A. & Clayton, N. S. (2015). Neophobia is not only avoidance: improving neophobia tests by combining cognition and ecology. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 6, 82–89.
      Griffin, A. S., Netto, K. & Peneaux, C. (2017). Neophilia, innovation and learning in an urbanized world: a critical evaluation of mixed findings. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 16, 15–22.
      Griffiths, T. L. & Tenenbaum, J. B. (2007). From mere coincidences to meaningful discoveries. Cognition 103(2), 180–226.
      Gross, M. E., Zedelius, C. M. & Schooler, J. W. (2020). Cultivating an understanding of curiosity as a seed for creativity. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 35, 77–82.
      Gruber, M. J. & Fandakova, Y. (2021). Curiosity in childhood and adolescence—what can we learn from the brain. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 39, 178–184.
      Gruber, M. J., Gelman, B. D. & Ranganath, C. (2014). States of curiosity modulate hippocampus‐dependent learning via the dopaminergic circuit. Neuron 84(2), 486–496.
      Gweon, H., Pelton, H., Konopka, J. A. & Schulz, L. E. (2014). Sins of omission: children selectively explore when teachers are under‐informative. Cognition 132(3), 335–341.
      Halina, M. (2015). There is no special problem of mindreading in nonhuman animals. Philosophy of Science 82(3), 473–490.
      Hall, B. A., Melfi, V., Burns, A., Mcgill, D. M. & Doyle, R. E. (2018). Curious creatures: a multi‐taxa investigation of responses to novelty in a zoo environment. PeerJ 6, e4454.
      Hardy, J. H. III, Ness, A. M. & Mecca, J. (2017). Outside the box: epistemic curiosity as a predictor of creative problem solving and creative performance. Personality and Individual Differences 104, 230–237.
      Harris, P. L. & Lane, J. D. (2014). Infants understand how testimony works. Topoi 33, 443–458.
      Hausberger, M., Henry, L., Rethore, B., Pougnault, L., Kremers, D., Rössler, C., Aubry, C., Cousillas, H., Boye, M. & Lemasson, A. (2021). When perceptual laterality vanishes with curiosity: a study in dolphins and starlings. Laterality 26(1–2), 238–259.
      Heinrich, B. (1995). Neophilia and exploration in juvenile common ravens, Corvus corax. Animal Behaviour 50(3), 695–704.
      Hembacher, E., Demayo, B. & Frank, M. C. (2020). Children's social information seeking is sensitive to referential ambiguity. Child Development 91(6), e1178–e1193.
      Herrmann, E., Hare, B., Cissewski, J. & Tomasello, M. (2011). A comparison of temperament in nonhuman apes and human infants. Developmental Science 14(6), 1393–1405.
      Horwitz, J. C. (2000). Mesolimbocortical and nigrostriatal dopamine responses to salient non‐reward events. Neuroscience 96(4), 651–656.
      Huber, L., Rechberger, S. & Taborsky, M. (2001). Social learning affects object exploration and manipulation in keas, Nestor notabilis. Animal Behaviour 62(5), 945–954.
      Inglis, I. R., Forkman, B. & Lazarus, J. (1997). Free food or earned food? A review and fuzzy model of contrafreeloading. Animal Behaviour 53(6), 1171–1191.
      Inzani, E. L., Kelley, L. A. & Boogert, N. J. (2022). Object neophilia in wild herring gulls in urban and rural locations. Journal of Avian Biology 1–2, e03028.
      Jaeggi, A. V., Dunkel, L. P., Van Noordwijk, M. A., Wich, S. A., Sura, A. A. & van Schaik, C. P. (2010). Social learning of diet and foraging skills by wild immature Bornean orangutans: implications for culture. American Journal of Primatology 72(1), 62–71.
      Jaeggi, A. V., van Noordwijk, M. A. & van Schaik, C. P. (2008). Begging for information: mother–offspring food sharing among wild Bornean orangutans. American Journal of Primatology 70(6), 533–541.
      James, W. (1890). The principles of Psychology Volume II.
      Jepma, M., Verdonschot, R. G., Van Steenbergen, H., Rombouts, S. A. & Nieuwenhuis, S. (2012). Neural mechanisms underlying the induction and relief of perceptual curiosity. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience 6, 5.
      Kano, F. & Tomonaga, M. (2009). How chimpanzees look at pictures: a comparative eye‐tracking study. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 276(1664), 1949–1955.
      Karl, S., Boch, M., Zamansky, A., van der Linden, D., Wagner, I. C., Völter, C. J., Lamm, C. & Huber, L. (2020). Exploring the dog–human relationship by combining fMRI, eye‐tracking and behavioural measures. Scientific Reports 10(1), 22273.
      Kaulfuß, P. & Mills, D. S. (2008). Neophilia in domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) and its implication for studies of dog cognition. Animal Cognition 11, 553–556.
      Kelleher, S. R., Silla, A. J. & Byrne, P. G. (2018). Animal personality and behavioral syndromes in amphibians: a review of the evidence, experimental approaches, and implications for conservation. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 72(5), 1–26.
      Kelley, A. E., Schochef, T. & Landry, C. F. (2004). Risk taking and novelty seeking in adolescence: introduction to part I. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1021(1), 27–32.
      Kelly, K. T. (2004). Justification as truth‐finding efficiency: how Ockham's razor works. Minds and Machines 14, 485–505.
      Kendal, R. L., Coe, R. L. & Laland, K. N. (2005). Age differences in neophilia, exploration, and innovation in family groups of callitrichid monkeys. American Journal of Primatology 66(2), 167–188.
      Kidd, C. & Hayden, B. Y. (2015). The psychology and neuroscience of curiosity. Neuron 88(3), 449–460.
      Kidd, C., Piantadosi, S. T. & Aslin, R. N. (2012). The goldilocks effect: human infants allocate attention to visual sequences that are neither too simple nor too complex. PLoS One 7, e36399.
      Kinzler, K. D., Dupoux, E. & Spelke, E. S. (2007). The native language of social cognition. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 104(30), 12577–12580.
      Kita, S. (ed.) (2003). Pointing: Where Language, Culture, and Cognition Meet. Psychology Press, New Jersey.
      Kovacs, A. M., Tauzin, T., Teglas, E., Gergely, G. & Csibra, G. (2014). Pointing as epistemic request: 12‐month‐olds point to receive new information. Infancy 19(6), 543–557.
      Kulkarni, T. D., Narasimhan, K., Saeedi, A. & Tenenbaum, J. (2016). Hierarchical deep reinforcement learning: integrating temporal abstraction and intrinsic motivation. Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems 29, 3675–3683.
      Kummer, H. & Goodall, J. (1985). Conditions of innovative behaviour in primates. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. B, Biological Sciences 308(1135), 203–214.
      Lachman, S. J. (1997). Learning is a process: toward an improved definition of learning. The Journal of Psychology 131(5), 477–480.
      Langston, R. F., Stevenson, C. H., Wilson, C. L., Saunders, I. & Wood, E. R. (2010). The role of hippocampal subregions in memory for stimulus associations. Behavioural Brain Research 215(2), 275–291.
      Laurent, P. A. (2008). The emergence of saliency and novelty responses from reinforcement learning principles. Neural Networks 21(10), 1493–1499.
      Lee, N., Lazaro, V., Wang, J. J., Sen, H. H. & Lucca, K. (2023). Exploring individual differences in infants' looking preferences for impossible events: the early multidimensional curiosity scale. Frontiers in Psychology 13, 1015649.
      Leslie, A. M., Friedman, O. & German, T. P. (2004). Core mechanisms in ‘theory of mind’. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 8(12), 528–533.
      Lesort, T., Lomonaco, V., Stoian, A., Maltoni, D., Filliat, D. & Diaz‐Rodriguez, N. (2020). Continual learning for robotics: definition, framework, learning strategies, opportunities and challenges. Information Fusion 58, 52–68.
      Levin, M. (2019). The computational boundary of a ‘self’: developmental bioelectricity drives multicellularity and scale‐free cognition. Frontiers in Psychology 10, 2688.
      Lewis, L. S. & Krupenye, C. (2022). Eye‐tracking as a window into primate social cognition. American Journal of Primatology 84(10), e23393.
      Loewenstein, G. (1994). The psychology of curiosity: a review and reinterpretation. Psychological Bulletin 116(1), 75–98.
      Lucca, K. (2020). The development of information‐requesting gestures in infancy and their role in shaping learning outcomes. In The Questioning Child: Insights from Psychology and Education (eds L. P. Butler, S. Ronfard and K. H. Corriveau), pp. 89–117. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK.
      Lucca, K. & Wilbourn, M. P. (2018). Communicating to learn: Infants' pointing gestures result in optimal learning. Child Development 89(3), 941–960.
      Lucca, K. & Wilbourn, M. P. (2019). The what and the how: information‐seeking pointing gestures facilitate learning labels and functions. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 178, 417–436.
      Lungarella, M., Metta, G., Pfeifer, R. & Sandini, G. (2003). Developmental robotics: a survey. Connection Science 15, 151–190.
      Luo, Y. & Baillargeon, R. (2005). Can a self‐propelled box have a goal? Psychological reasoning in 5‐month‐old infants. Psychological Science 16(8), 601–608.
      Lyons, K. E. & Ghetti, S. (2011). The development of uncertainty monitoring in early childhood. Child Development 82(6), 1778–1787.
      Markey, A. & Loewenstein, G. (2014). Curiosity. In International Handbook of Emotions in Education (eds R. Pekrun and L. Linnenbrink‐Garcia), pp. 238–255. Routledge, New York.
      Martins, C. I., Schaedelin, F. C., Mann, M., Blum, C., Mandl, I., Urban, D., Grill, J., Schösswender, J. & Wagner, R. H. (2012). Exploring novelty: a component trait of behavioural syndromes in a colonial fish. Behaviour 149(2), 215–231.
      Marvin, C. B., Tedeschi, E. & Shohamy, D. (2020). Curiosity as the impulse to know: common behavioral and neural mechanisms underlying curiosity and impulsivity. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 35, 92–98.
      Massen, J. J., Antonides, A., Arnold, A. M. K., Bionda, T. & Koski, S. E. (2013). A behavioral view on chimpanzee personality: exploration tendency, persistence, boldness, and tool‐orientation measured with group experiments. American Journal of Primatology 75(9), 947–958.
      McCrink, K. & Wynn, K. (2004). Large‐number addition and subtraction by 9‐month‐old infants. Psychological Science 15(11), 776–781.
      Mettke‐Hofmann, C. (2014). Cognitive ecology: ecological factors, life‐styles, and cognition. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science 5(3), 345–360.
      Mettke‐Hofmann, C., Rowe, K. C., Hayden, T. J. & Canonine, V. (2006). Effects of experience and object complexity on exploration in garden warblers (Sylvia borin). Journal of Zoology 268(4), 405–413.
      Mikhalevich, I., Powell, R. & Logan, C. (2017). Is behavioural flexibility evidence of cognitive complexity? How evolution can inform comparative cognition. Interface Focus 7(3), 20160121.
      Miller, R., Lambert, M. L., Frohnwieser, A., Brecht, K. F., Bugnyar, T., Crampton, I., Garcia‐Pelegrin, E., Gould, K., Greggor, A. L., Izawa, E.‐I., Kelly, D. M., Zhongqiu, L., Yunchao, L., Linh, B. L., Massen, J. J., et al. (2022). Socio‐ecological correlates of neophobia in corvids. Current Biology 32(1), 74–85.
      Mirolli, M. & Baldassarre, G. (2013). Functions and mechanisms of intrinsic motivations. In Intrinsically Motivated Learning in Natural and Artificial Systems, pp. 49–72. Springer Berlin, Heidelberg.
      Moretti, L., Hentrup, M., Kotrschal, K. & Range, F. (2015). The influence of relationships on neophobia and exploration in wolves and dogs. Animal Behaviour 107, 159–173.
      Morgan, C. L. (1903). An introduction to comparative psychology, New ed., rev.
      Nakano, T., Watananbe, H., Homae, F. & Taga, G. (2009). Prefrontal cortical involvement in young infants' analysis of novelty. Cerebral Cortex 19(2), 455–463.
      Nissen, H. W. (1931). A study of exploratory behavior in the white rat by means of the obstruction method. In Animal Motivation Experimental Studies on the Albino Rat, pp. 351–368. Columbia University Press, New York.
      Noordewier, M. K. & van Dijk, E. (2017). Curiosity and time: from not knowing to almost knowing. Cognition and Emotion 31(3), 411–421.
      Nyborg, H. (ed.) (2003). The Scientific Study of General Intelligence: Tribute to Arthur Jensen. Elsevier Science, Oxford, UK.
      O'Hara, M., Mioduszewska, B., von Bayern, A., Auersperg, A., Bugnyar, T., Wilkinson, A., Huber, L. & Gajdon, G. K. (2017). The temporal dependence of exploration on neotic style in birds. Scientific Reports 7(1), 4742.
      Oudeyer, P. Y. & Kaplan, F. (2007). What is intrinsic motivation? A typology of computational approaches. Frontiers in Neurorobotics 1, 6.
      Oudeyer, P. Y., Kaplan, F. & Hafner, V. V. (2007). Intrinsic motivation systems for autonomous mental development. IEEE Transactions on Evolutionary Computation 11, 265–286.
      Oudeyer, P. Y. & Smith, L. B. (2016). How evolution may work through curiosity‐driven developmental process. Topics in Cognitive Science 8(2), 492–502.
      Parisi, G. I., Tani, J., Weber, C. & Wermter, S. (2018). Lifelong learning of spatiotemporal representations with dual‐memory recurrent self‐organization. Frontiers in Neurorobotics 12, 78.
      Pathak, D., Agrawal, P., Efros, A. A. & Darrell, T. (2017). Curiosity‐driven exploration by self‐supervised prediction. In International Conference on Machine Learning, pp. 2778–2787. PMLR, Sydney.
      Pellegrini, A. D. & Smith, P. K. (eds) (2005). The Nature of Play: Great Apes and Humans. Guilford Press, New York.
      Perez, J. & Feigenson, L. (2021). Stable individual differences in infants' responses to violations of intuitive physics. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 118(27), e2103805118.
      Perez, J. & Feigenson, L. (2022). Violations of expectation trigger infants to search for explanations. Cognition 218, 104942.
      Pfeifer, R. & Iida, F. (2004). Embodied artificial intelligence: trends and challenges. Embodied Artificial Intelligence 3139, 1–26.
      Piantadosi, S. T., Jara‐Ettinger, J. & Gibson, E. (2014). Children's learning of number words in an indigenous farming‐foraging group. Developmental Science 17(4), 553–563.
      Pisula, W. (2020). Curiosity and Information Seeking in Animal and Human Behavior: A Review the Literature and Data in Comparative Psychology, Animal Cognition, Ethology, Ontogenesis, and Elements of Cognitive Neuroscience as they Relate to Animal Inquisitiveness. Universal Publishers, Irvine, CA.
      Pisula, W., Turlejski, K. & Charles, E. P. (2013). Comparative psychology as unified psychology: the case of curiosity and other novelty‐related behavior. Review of General Psychology 17(2), 224–229.
      Pisula, W., Turlejski, K., Stryjek, R., Nalecz‐Tolak, A., Grabiec, M. & Djavadian, R. L. (2012). Response to novelty in the laboratory Wistar rat, wild‐captive WWCPS rat, and the gray short‐tailed opossum (Monodelphis domestica). Behavioural Processes 91(2), 145–151.
      Powell, S. B., Geyer, M. A., Gallagher, D. & Paulus, M. P. (2004). The balance between approach and avoidance behaviors in a novel object exploration paradigm in mice. Behavioural Brain Research 152(2), 341–349.
      Quinn, P. C., Yahr, J., Kuhn, A., Slater, A. M. & Pascalis, O. (2002). Representation of the gender of human faces by infants: a preference for female. Perception 31(9), 1109–1121.
      Reader, S. M. (2015). Causes of individual differences in animal exploration and search. Topics in Cognitive Science 7(3), 451–468.
      Redolat, R., Perez‐Martinez, A., Carrasco, M. C. & Mesa, P. (2009). Individual differences in novelty‐seeking and behavioral responses to nicotine: a review of animal studies. Current Drug Abuse Reviews 2(3), 230–242.
      Reynolds, G. D. (2015). Infant visual attention and object recognition. Behavioural Brain Research 285, 34–43.
      Rojas‐Ferrer, I., Thompson, M. J. & Morand‐Ferron, J. (2020). Is exploration a metric for information gathering? Attraction to novelty and plasticity in black‐capped chickadees. Ethology 126(4), 383–392.
      Ronfard, S., Zambrana, I. M., Hermansen, T. K. & Kelemen, D. (2018). Question‐asking in childhood: a review of the literature and a framework for understanding its development. Developmental Review 49, 101–120.
      Rössler, T., Mioduszewska, B., O'Hara, M., Huber, L., Prawiradilag, D. M. & Auersperg, A. M. (2020). Using an innovation arena to compare wild‐caught and laboratory Goffin's cockatoos. Scientific Reports 10(1), 8681.
      Roth, T. C., Ladage, L. D. & Pravosudov, V. V. (2010). Learning capabilities enhanced in harsh environments: a common garden approach. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 277(1697), 3187–3193.
      Russell, J. L., Lyn, H., Schaeffer, J. A. & Hopkins, W. D. (2011). The role of socio‐communicative rearing environments in the development of social and physical cognition in apes. Developmental Science 14(6), 1459–1470.
      Santucci, V. G., Baldassarre, G. & Cartoni, E. (2019). Autonomous reinforcement learning of multiple interrelated tasks. In Joint IEEE 9th International Conference on Development and Learning and Epigenetic Robotics (ICDL‐EpiRob), pp. 221–227. IEEE, Oslo.
      Santucci, V. G., Baldassarre, G. & Mirolli, M. (2014). Cumulative learning through intrinsic reinforcements. In Evolution, Complexity and Artificial Life, pp. 107–122. Springer Berlin, Heidelberg.
      Santucci, V. G., Oudeyer, P. Y., Barto, A. & Baldassarre, G. (2020). Editorial: intrinsically motivated open‐ended learning in autonomous robots. Frontiers in Neurorobotics 13, 115.
      Schillaci, G., Ciria, A. & Lara, B. (2020). Tracking emotions: intrinsic motivation grounded on multilevel prediction error dynamics. In 2020 Joint IEEE 10th International Conference on Development and Learning and Epigenetic Robotics (ICDL‐EpiRob). IEEE, Valparaiso, Chile.
      Schmidhuber, J. (1991a). A possibility for implementing curiosity and boredom in model‐building neural controllers. In Proceedings of the International Conference on Simulation of Adaptive Behavior: From Animals to Animats, pp. 222–227. MIT Press/Bradford Books, Cambridge, MA.
      Schmidhuber, J. (1991b). Curious model‐building control systems. In Proceedings of the International Joint Conference on Neural Networks, pp. 1458–1463. IEEE, Singapore.
      Schultz, L. E. & Bonawitz, E. B. (2007). Serious fun: preschoolers engage in more exploratory play when evidence is confounded. Developmental Psychology 43(4), 1045–1050.
      Schultz, W. (2016). Dopamine reward prediction‐error signalling: a two‐component response. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 17(3), 183–195.
      Schuppli, C., Forss, S. I. F., Meulman, E. J., Zweifel, N., Lee, K. C., Rukumana, E., Vogel, E. R., van Noordwijk, M. A. & van Schaik, C. P. (2016). Development of foraging skills in two orangutan populations: needing to learn or needing to grow? Frontiers in Zoology 13, 1–17.
      Schwartz, C. E., Kunwar, P. S., Grev, D. N., Moran, L. R., Viner, J. C., Covino, J. M., Kagan, J., Stewart, E., Snidman, N. C., Vangel, M. G. & Wallace, S. R. (2010). Structural differences in adult orbital and ventromedial prefrontal cortex predicted by infant temperament at 4 months of age. Archives of General Psychiatry 67(1), 78–84.
      Shettleworth, S. J. (2010). Clever animals and killjoy explanations in comparative psychology. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 14(11), 477–481.
      Shettleworth, S. J. (2012). Darwin, Tinbergen, and the evolution of comparative cognition. In The Oxford Handbook of Comparative Evolutionary Psychology, pp. 529–546. Elsevier Publishing, Amsterdam.
      Shin, D. D. & Kim, S. I. (2019). Homo curious: curious or interested? Educational Psychology Review 31, 853–874.
      Silvia, P. J. (2008). Interest—the curious emotion. Current Directions in Psychological Science 17(1), 57–60.
      Sim, Z. L. & Xu, F. (2017). Infants preferentially approach and explore the unexpected. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 35(4), 596–608.
      Sim, Z. L. & Xu, F. (2019). Another look at looking time: surprise as rational statistical inference. Topics in Cognitive Science 11(1), 154–163.
      Siviter, H., Charles Deeming, D., Rosenberger, J., Burman, O. H., Moszuti, S. A. & Wilkinson, A. (2017). The impact of egg incubation temperature on the personality of oviparous reptiles. Animal Cognition 20(1), 109–116.
      Sober, E. (2015). Is the scientific method a myth? Perspectives from the history and philosophy of science. METODE Science Studies Journal 5, 195–199.
      Stahl, A. E. & Feigenson, L. (2015). Observing the unexpected enhances infants' learning and exploration. Science 348, 91–94.
      Sutton, R. S. (1991). Reinforcement learning architectures for animats. In Proceedings of the First International Conference on Simulation of Adaptive Behavior on from Animals to Animats, pp. 288–296. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.
      Sutton, R. S. & Barto, A. G. (2018). Reinforcement Learning: An Introduction. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.
      Szabo, B. & Ringler, E. (2023). Fear of the new? Geckos hesitate to attack novel prey, feed near objects and enter a novel space. Animal Cognition 26(2), 537–549.
      Takola, E., Krause, E. T., Müller, C. & Schielzeth, H. (2021). Novelty at second glance: a critical appraisal of the novel object paradigm based on meta‐analysis. Animal Behaviour 180, 123–142.
      Taylor, M., Esbensen, B. M. & Bennett, R. T. (1994). Children's understanding of knowledge acquisition: the tendency for children to report that they have always known what they have just learned. Child Development 65(6), 1581–1604.
      Teglas, E., Girotto, V., Gonzales, M. & Bonatti, L. L. (2007). Intuitions of probabilities shape expectations about the future at 12 months and beyond. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 104(48), 19156–19159.
      Thorndike, E. (1911). Animal intelligence: Experimental studies. Macmillan Press, New York.
      Tian, X., Silva, A. C. & Liu, C. (2021). The brain circuits and dynamics of curiosity‐driven behavior in naturally curious marmosets. Cerebral Cortex 31(9), 4220–4232.
      Tinbergen, N. (1963). On aims and methods of ethology. Zeitschrift für tierpsychologie 20, 410–433.
      Tomasello, M. & Call, J. (2004). The role of humans in the cognitive development of apes revisited. Animal Cognition 7, 213–215.
      Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M. & Liszkowski, U. (2007). A new look at infant pointing. Child Development 78(3), 705–722.
      Turati, C., Simion, F., Milani, I. & Umilta, C. (2002). Newborns' preference for faces: what is crucial? Developmental Psychology 38(6), 875–882.
      Twomey, K. E. & Westermann, G. (2018). Curiosity‐based learning in infants: a neurocomputational approach. Developmental Science 21(4), e12629.
      Valenza, E., Simion, F., Cassia, V. M. & Umilta, C. (1996). Face preference at birth. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 22(4), 892–903.
      Valenzo, D., Ciria, A., Schillaci, G. & Lara, B. (2022). Grounding context in embodied cognitive robotics. Frontiers in Neurorobotics 16, 843108.
      van de Waal, E. & Bshary, R. (2011). Contact with human facilities appears to enhance technical skills in wild vervet monkeys (Chlorocebus aethiops). Folia Primatologica 81(5), 282–291.
      van Schaik, C. P., Burkart, J., Damerius, L., Forss, S. I., Koops, K., van Noordwijk, M. A. & Schuppli, C. (2016). The reluctant innovator: orangutans and the phylogeny of creativity. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 371(1690), 20150183.
      Wang, M. Z. & Hayden, B. Y. (2019). Monkeys are curious about counterfactual outcomes. Cognition 189, 1–10.
      Wang, M. Z. & Hayden, B. Y. (2021). Latent learning, cognitive maps, and curiosity. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 38, 1–7.
      Wang, S. H., Baillargeon, R. & Brueckner, L. (2004). Young infants' reasoning about hidden objects: evidence from violation‐of‐expectation tasks with test trials only. Cognition 93(3), 167–198.
      Wang, S. H., Zhang, Y. & Baillargeon, R. (2016). Young infants view physically possible support events as unexpected: new evidence for rule learning. Cognition 157, 100–105.
      Warner, M. (2007). Sources and methods for the study of intelligence. In Handbook of intelligence studies, pp. 35–45. Routledge Press, New York.
      Washburne, J. N. (1936). The definition of learning. Journal of Educational Psychology 27(8), 603–611.
      Wendler, D. (2014). Should protections for research with humans who cannot consent apply to research with nonhuman primates? Theoretical Medicine and Bioethics 35, 157–173.
      Werchan, D. M., Collins, A. G., Frank, M. J. & Amso, D. (2016). Role of prefrontal cortex in learning and generalizing hierarchical rules in 8‐month‐old infants. Journal of Neuroscience 36(40), 10314–10322.
      White, R. W. (1959). Motivation reconsidered: the concept of competence. Psychological Review 66, 297–333.
      Wittmann, B. C., Bunzeck, N., Dolan, R. J. & Düzel, E. (2007). Anticipation of novelty recruits reward system and hippocampus while promoting recollection. NeuroImage 38(1), 194–202.
      Wu, Y. & Gweon, H. (2019). Surprisingly unsurprising! Infants' looking time at probable vs. improbable events is modulated by others' expressions of surprise. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/8whuv.
      Wynn, K. (1992). Children's acquisition of the number words and the counting system. Cognitive Psychology 24(2), 220–251.
      Wynne, C. D. (2004). The perils of anthropomorphism. Nature 428(6983), 606.
      Xiong, F., Liu, Z., Huan, K., Yang, X. & Qiao, H. (2021). State primitive learning to overcome catastrophic forgetting in robotics. Cognitive Computation 13, 394–402.
      Zettersten, M. & Saffran, J. R. (2021). Sampling to learn words: adults and children sample words that reduce referential ambiguity. Developmental Science 24(3), e13064.
      Zhai, X., Puigcercer, J., Kolesnikov, A., Ruyssen, P., Riquelme, C., Lucic, M., Djolonga, J., Susano Pinto, A., Neumann, M., Dosovitskiy, A., Beyer, L., Bachem, O., Tschannen, M., Michalski, M., Bousquet, O., et al. (2019). A large‐scale study of representation learning with the visual task adaptation benchmark. arXiv preprint arXiv:1910.04867.
    • Grant Information:
      PZ00P3_202052/2 Schweizerischer Nationalfonds zur Förderung der Wissenschaftlichen Forschung
    • Contributed Indexing:
      Keywords: animal cognition; computational cognition; curiosity; developmental psychology; exploration; information processing; intrinsic motivation
    • Publication Date:
      Date Created: 20240129 Date Completed: 20240507 Latest Revision: 20240628
    • Publication Date:
      20240629
    • Accession Number:
      10.1111/brv.13054
    • Accession Number:
      38287201