Leadership dynamics in musical groups: Quantifying effects of musical structure on directionality of influence in concert performance videos.

Item request has been placed! ×
Item request cannot be made. ×
loading   Processing Request
  • Additional Information
    • Source:
      Publisher: Public Library of Science Country of Publication: United States NLM ID: 101285081 Publication Model: eCollection Cited Medium: Internet ISSN: 1932-6203 (Electronic) Linking ISSN: 19326203 NLM ISO Abbreviation: PLoS One Subsets: MEDLINE
    • Publication Information:
      Original Publication: San Francisco, CA : Public Library of Science
    • Subject Terms:
    • Abstract:
      Music ensemble performance provides an ecologically valid context for investigating leadership dynamics in small group interactions. Musical texture, specifically the relative salience of simultaneously sounding ensemble parts, is a feature that can potentially alter leadership dynamics by introducing hierarchical relationships between individual parts. The present study extended previous work on quantifying interpersonal coupling in musical ensembles by examining the relationship between musical texture and leader-follower relations, operationalised as directionality of influence between co-performers' body motion in concert video recordings. It was hypothesised that the directionality of influence, indexed by Granger Causality, would be greater for 'homophonic' textures with a clear distinction between melody and accompaniment parts than for 'polyphonic' textures with less distinction between melody and accompaniment. This hypothesis was tested by using pose estimation algorithms to track instrumentalists' body movements in a string quartet and a clarinet quintet, and then applying Granger Causality analysis to their head motion to estimate directional influence between instrumentalist pairs for sections of the pieces that varied in texture. It was found that Granger Causality values were generally higher (indicating greater directionality of influence) for homophonic than polyphonic textures. Furthermore, considering melody and accompaniment instrument roles revealed more evidence for the melody instrument influencing accompanying instruments than vice versa, plus a high degree of directionality among accompanying instruments, in homophonic textures. These observed patterns of directional information flow in co-performer body motion are consistent with changing leader-follower relations depending on hierarchical relations between ensemble parts in terms of the relative salience of melodic material in the musical texture. The finding that automatic pose estimation can detect modulations of leadership dynamics in standard video recordings under naturalistic performance conditions has implications for investigating interpersonal coordination in large-scale music video datasets representing different cultural traditions, and for exploring nonverbal communication in group activities more generally.
      Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.
      (Copyright: © 2024 Sabharwal et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.)
    • References:
      Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2021 Sep;128:661-677. (PMID: 34273378)
      PLoS One. 2014 Jan 24;9(1):e84085. (PMID: 24475030)
      Sci Rep. 2019 Apr 10;9(1):5854. (PMID: 30971783)
      Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2021 Oct 11;376(1835):20200332. (PMID: 34420393)
      Front Psychol. 2022 Nov 01;13:987775. (PMID: 36389557)
      Neuroimage. 2014 Jul 1;94:79-88. (PMID: 24631790)
      Psychol Sci. 2007 Sep;18(9):756-7. (PMID: 17760767)
      Front Psychol. 2013 Nov 13;4:841. (PMID: 24312065)
      J R Soc Interface. 2014 Jan 29;11(93):20131125. (PMID: 24478285)
      Front Physiol. 2012 Oct 19;3:405. (PMID: 23091463)
      Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2023 Apr 24;378(1875):20210477. (PMID: 36871583)
      PLoS One. 2016 Apr 22;11(4):e0154322. (PMID: 27104377)
      Adv Methods Pract Psychol Sci. 2021 Jan-Mar;4(1):. (PMID: 35821764)
      Front Psychol. 2018 Jul 24;9:1285. (PMID: 30087645)
      Emotion. 2016 Apr;16(3):365-80. (PMID: 26501928)
      Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2016 May 5;371(1693):. (PMID: 27069054)
      Behav Res Methods. 2023 Feb;55(2):932-962. (PMID: 35513768)
      Q J Exp Psychol A. 2004 Apr;57(3):499-521. (PMID: 15204138)
      Neuropsychologia. 2014 Mar;55:98-104. (PMID: 24333167)
      Trends Cogn Sci. 2006 Feb;10(2):70-6. (PMID: 16406326)
      Front Hum Neurosci. 2022 Sep 29;16:899676. (PMID: 36248684)
      Trends Cogn Sci. 2015 Mar;19(3):111-4. (PMID: 25641075)
      Front Psychol. 2021 Mar 16;12:590987. (PMID: 33828503)
      Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2017 May 23;114(21):E4134-E4141. (PMID: 28484007)
      Neuropsychologia. 2023 May 3;183:108524. (PMID: 36868500)
      Psychol Res. 2018 Nov;82(6):1177-1194. (PMID: 28733769)
      Music Percept. 2020 Feb;37(3):185-195. (PMID: 36936548)
      Trends Cogn Sci. 2023 Nov;27(11):1008-1018. (PMID: 37277276)
      Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011 Dec 27;108(52):20947-52. (PMID: 22160696)
      R Soc Open Sci. 2015 Nov 18;2(11):150286. (PMID: 26715993)
      Front Hum Neurosci. 2018 Oct 08;12:401. (PMID: 30349467)
      Neuropsychologia. 2016 Aug;89:414-425. (PMID: 27449708)
      Neural Netw. 2010 Oct-Nov;23(8-9):998-1003. (PMID: 20598504)
      Sci Rep. 2019 Jan 18;9(1):205. (PMID: 30659220)
      Sci Rep. 2019 Aug 22;9(1):12255. (PMID: 31439866)
      Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2014 Dec 19;369(1658):20130394. (PMID: 25385772)
      J R Soc Interface. 2017 Oct;14(135):. (PMID: 29021158)
      R Soc Open Sci. 2018 Feb 21;5(2):171520. (PMID: 29515867)
      Front Psychol. 2021 Apr 27;12:638783. (PMID: 33986708)
      Front Psychol. 2014 Jun 23;5:645. (PMID: 25002856)
      Front Psychol. 2015 Apr 20;6:313. (PMID: 25941499)
      Perception. 2014;43(8):825-8. (PMID: 25549513)
      Sci Rep. 2022 Jan 10;12(1):421. (PMID: 35013620)
      Front Psychol. 2013 Jun 25;4:368. (PMID: 23805116)
      Sci Rep. 2021 Sep 15;11(1):18379. (PMID: 34526559)
      Neuroimage. 2020 Feb 1;206:116303. (PMID: 31654761)
      Conscious Cogn. 2019 Feb;68:73-96. (PMID: 30660927)
      PLoS Comput Biol. 2019 Oct 16;15(10):e1007422. (PMID: 31618261)
      Elife. 2022 Nov 01;11:. (PMID: 36317963)
      Front Hum Neurosci. 2012 Jun 07;6:164. (PMID: 22701413)
      Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2022 Oct;1516(1):106-113. (PMID: 35819164)
      Q J Exp Psychol (Hove). 2018 Jul;71(7):1535-1551. (PMID: 28585902)
      Atten Percept Psychophys. 2014 Feb;76(2):527-40. (PMID: 24170378)
      Front Hum Neurosci. 2013 Jun 10;7:253. (PMID: 23772211)
      PeerJ. 2018 May 23;6:e4794. (PMID: 29844961)
      Front Psychol. 2020 Dec 10;11:573030. (PMID: 33362639)
      PLoS One. 2012;7(5):e35757. (PMID: 22590511)
      Psychol Res. 2011 Sep;75(5):423-34. (PMID: 20981438)
      Q J Exp Psychol (Hove). 2019 Sep;72(9):2272-2287. (PMID: 30744490)
      Acta Psychol (Amst). 2021 Jul;218:103348. (PMID: 34058671)
    • Publication Date:
      Date Created: 20240403 Date Completed: 20240405 Latest Revision: 20240406
    • Publication Date:
      20240406
    • Accession Number:
      PMC10990194
    • Accession Number:
      10.1371/journal.pone.0300663
    • Accession Number:
      38568939